Today we're looking at the article "Recursive syntactic pattern learning in songbirds" by Timothy Genter, Kimberly Fenn, Daniel Margoliashi, and Howard Nusbaum, which was published in Nature 440 in 2006.1 The article details a small study done on European starlings (Sturnus vulgaris, pictured), a songbird native to Europe and also found in North America following human introduction. According to All About Birds, published by the Cornell Lab or Ornithology, the starling is capable of producing a wide range of vocalizations, including warbles, whistles, chatter, trills, rattles, and mimicry of other birds, though this study focuses only on warble and rattle motifs.2
Historically, linguists have theorized that all non-human languages can be categorized as having finite-state grammar—in other words, animals can only use regular languages. What makes human language unique, then, is the ability to use context-free grammar, which allows for recursion, also known as embedding, a process of inserting a syntactic structure into another. For example, it's easy for us to understand the sentence "I ate the apple that fell from the tree that they cut down", even though it includes clauses embedded inside clauses. Although the general theory has been that animal languages cannot accommodate this type of construction, some linguists maintain that some recursion is indeed possible in animal language, and that non-human creatures may not be strictly limited to languages that can expressed through a finite-state automaton.
To test this hypothesis, researchers trained 11 European starlings to distinguish between warble/rattle patterns of the form (AB)n (regular) and patterns of the form AnBn (non-regular), where A is a recording of a warble and B a recording of a rattle. Figure 1 shows examples of these pattern structures, while Figure 2a and 2b show spectrogram analysis of specific constructions. Of the 11 starlings, 9 were successfully able to distinguish between them, with some learning faster than others. Thus the results were sorted into three groups: fast learning, slow learning, and non-learning, as shown in Figure 2c and 2d. In these graph, d' represents the ability to distinguish between the patterns. This strongly suggests that starlings have the capacity to understand context-free languages, and not only regular languages.
Although the sample group was fairly small, it seems unlikely that these starlings were the only 9 starlings in the world capable of this distinction. Moreover, it seems unlikely that starlings are the only birds—or even the only animals—with the ability to recognize and extrapolate from context-free structures. This could have strong implications for biolinguistics; after all, this result forces us to wonder what it really is that makes human language unique. We also wonder whether starlings have the facilities to use these language features in the wild, or can merely recognize them in a trained laboratory setting.
About our team
Our team for this project was Julia Chen, Katrina Kuo, Alice Mu, Max Ranieri. Max, being a linguistics minor, chose the topic, and vetted it with Katrina to make sure that it was actually interesting and accessible to a wider audience. Then, the group met to discuss the topic. After reviewing the article together, we talked about its implications and helped each other to understand the research methods and real-world language background. Julia further researched context-free and finite-state grammars, while Alice prepared some thoughts on the takeaways for the paper. Meanwhile, Max and Katrina designed an activity to help our audience understand how the experiment was performed, and (hopefully) show a human's competence in the task of distinguishing between different types of patterns. Ultimately we decided to fairly randomly distribute presenting duties, with Alice covering the FSG/CFG background and explaining the results of the experiment, Max talking about the research methods and takeaways, Katrina explaining the figures and leading our activity, and Julia giving background on European starlings. It was easy to take on these roles as we all took an active role in preparing the presentation, talking through our contributions with each other before finalizing them. Finally, Max used their excellent web programming skills to create this web page.
Works cited
- Gentner, Timothy Q., et al. "Recursive Syntactic Pattern Learning by Songbirds." Nature, 440. 27 Apr. 2006.
- "European Starling". All About Birds. Cornell.
Further Reading
- Syntactic Structures by Noam Chomsky – For more linguistic structural background